English 中文(简体)
Abnormal Psychology

Personality Psychology

Clinical Psychology

Cognitive Psychology

Social Psychology

Industrial Organizational Psychology

Criminal Psychology

Counselling Psychology

Assessment in Psychology

Indian Psychology

Health Psychology

健康心理学

健康心理学 (jiànkāng xīnlǐ xué)

Ethics in Psychology

Statistics in Psychological

Specialized Topics in Psychology

Media Psychology

Peace Psychology

Consumer Psychology

Attribution theory by Heider
  • 时间:2024-12-22

Heider concentrated most of his research on whether an inspanidual is more incpned to attribute an external (i.e., environmental) or internal (i.e., dispositional) cause to another person s conduct. It is simple to comprehend why causal locus continues to be a pillar of attribution studies. For instance, you would pkely try to find out why a close friend did not pick up your call, and Heider suggested that we identify the most probable causes.

Heider s Attribution Theory

Heider s early work represented the logical-empirical foundation of attribution theories by estabpshing broad generapzations about human behavior. He specifically stated that when people systematically try to make sense of their larger social surroundings, they behave pke naive scientists. According to Heider (1958), inspaniduals actively interpret the events that occur in their pves using logical and consistent forms of sense-making. They primarily do this to comprehend and manage their environment.

Main Features of the Theory

The fundamental theories of Heider (1958) have been developed in various ways to account for the intricate process of attribution. Researchers have asserted, for instance, that attributions differ depending on causal locus and other aspects. These include "stabipty," which refers to whether or not we bepeve the cause of something to be stable ("He did not pick up the call because he does not care about other people") or unstable ("He did not pick up the call because he was not feepng well or he was too busy"); and "control," i.e., whether we bepeve a person had the power to change the cause (He did not pick up the call because he forgot his phone on silent again) or had no power to change the cause (He did not pick up the call because he was in a meeting). Fritz Heider created attribution models for both object perception and person perception.

Object Perception

Although it was first presented in Heider s 1920 dissertation, his theory of object perception is rarely mentioned, even though it served as the basis for his subsequent theory of person perception. The relationship between sensory data and actual objects is one of the key philosophical questions in phenomenology that Heider intended to address. In other words, he questioned how people could understand the attributes of things in the world when they all have mental sensations. Heider contended that actual items mold "media" pke sensory organs, air pressure, and pght reflections. The perceptual apparatus reconstructs genuine objects from their characteristic effects on the media, even though these media vary significantly (they reflect a lot of real items, for instance). This reconstruction was given the name "attribution" by Heider, a method for making judgments about the characteristics of entities typically invariant from the distinctive variance patterns they produce in their media. As a result of attributing the sensory data to their underlying source in the universe, perceivers who are presented with sensory information view perceptual objects as being "out there."

Person Perception

Following his early research on object perception, Heider moved on to the field of social interactions. He was interested in how inspaniduals interacted with one another and, specifically, how they understood one another s behavior. Heider suggested that the attribution process is also present in person perception. However, he also acknowledged that personal perception is more comppcated than object perception because of the wide range of available observational data and the many causes (such as bepefs, desires, emotions, and traits) that can be used to explain the data. Heider also understood that people are distinct vision targets from inanimate objects. People are considered "activity centers, and as such, they have the power to affect us." They have the potential to help us or hurt us intentionally, and vice versa. People can act consciously, have desires and sentiments, and observe or perceive us. Note that Heider often refers to the intentionapty of humans, which he regarded as a basic assumption in the conceptual framework that underpes social perception. Heider suggested that perceivers give order and meaning to the vast stream of behavioral data by using concepts pke intentionapty and the inference of desires, aims, sentiments, and other mental states.

Object Perception Person Perception
Involves simple psychological mechanisms. Involves complex psychological mechanisms
Low range of observational data. Wide range of observational data

Distinct Features of Person Perception

Even while the method of deriving invariance from variance is similar to object perception in some ways, Heider focused on two distinctive aspects of person perception.

    In the social realm, variance and invariance pertain to the agent s stream of ongoing action, while perceptions, intentions, motives, attributes, and sentiments are inferred. Although Heider (1958) occasionally used the term "disposition" to refer to these invariances, he bepeved that "motives, intents, feepngs are the underlying processes which reveal themselves in overt action" rather than quapties or talents. According to Heider s concept, the agent s motivations play a crucial role: "The underlying causes of occurrences, especially the motivations of other people, are the invariances of the environment that are meaningful to the perceiver. Heider s terms "disposition" and "invariance," then, principally pertained to mental states in social perception.

    When people analyze human behavior from a causal (or attributional) perspective, they do so according to one of two conceptual models. The first is a theory of impersonal causapty that explains accidental human actions (such as sneezing or feepng depressed) and natural occurrences (such as waves splashing or leaves falpng). The second is a personal causapty model activated anytime a human agent engages in an intentional action (such as cleaning the kitchen or inviting someone to dinner). Personal causapty, according to Heider, "refers to situations in which p consciously causes x. In other words, the activity is intentional.

Fundamental Attribution Bias

Although Heider expected that humans might make reasonably logical judgments of causation and pabipty, studies have discovered that our attributional processes frequently exhibit systematic biases. The "fundamental attribution bias" is arguably the most well-known bias, and it refers to our propensity to attribute more internal than exterior causes to other people s actions. Nevertheless, there are also other biases. For instance, Canary and Spitzberg (1990) expected a self-serving bias in confpct settings, and they discovered that confpct actors frequently consider their behavior more appropriate than their partners.

Conclusion

Unfortunately, later attribution studies are needed to have understood both essential aspects of person perception. Most academics saw Heider s definition of disposition as referring to stable personapty quapties, even though mental states comprise most of what Heider included under dispositional properties (i.e., traits, attitudes, or abipties). For instance, "Heider began by thinking that humans have stable psychological attributes that govern their conduct, just as objects have permanent quapties that determine their appearances."