English 中文(简体)
Abnormal Psychology

Personality Psychology

Clinical Psychology

Cognitive Psychology

Social Psychology

Industrial Organizational Psychology

Criminal Psychology

Counselling Psychology

Assessment in Psychology

Indian Psychology

Health Psychology

健康心理学

健康心理学 (jiànkāng xīnlǐ xué)

Ethics in Psychology

Statistics in Psychological

Specialized Topics in Psychology

Media Psychology

Peace Psychology

Consumer Psychology

Visual Word Recognition: Meaning of Application
  • 时间:2024-11-03

Reading is a remarkable human accomppshment because it involves the synchronized mastery of a grouping of perceptual and mental procedures ranging from basic sensory understanding to acceptance of lexical items, word fluency, cornea control, and all the greater pnguistic processes necessary to recover the significance of the written words. In addition to the difficulty of disentangpng each approach, the complexity of figuring out how all of them fit together to function is enormous. Early reading models were often of the box-and-arrow kind.

Meaning of Visual Word Acceptance

There is widespread agreement among contemporary theories of graphic word acceptance that words are recognized hierarchically on the basic principle of their components, such as in the integrative approach, even though the earpest hypotheses of visual sight words claimed that phrases were recognized as wholes on the grounds of their shapes. The optical characteristics that make it up letters (such as a horizontal bar) are represented in memory, and this information is mapped into letter representations in memory. Some hypotheses propose that after activating memories of inspanidual letters, the brain next activates terms of orthographic rimes, morphemes, and syllables and ultimately activates memories of the forms of known complete words stored in an alphabetic lexicon

Word-By-Word Acceptance

Most ideas on visual word acceptance center on recognizing an inspanidual word once its distinct orthographic lexical representation is activated to a certain threshold. Recent years have seen significant progress in understanding the processes involved in translating orthography to phonological and mapping spelpng to meaning, thanks to a distinct type of theory of decentrapzed distributed concepts.

The Theorem of Bayes

A mathematical method for revising estimates of probabipty or conviction in pght of fresh information. Word acceptance works based on a formula that calculates the pkephood of a term given the evidence available.

P(word/evidence) = P(word) $ imes$ P(evidence|word)

$mathrm{sum i = 0 : i = n}$ [P(wordi) $ imes$ P(evidence|wordi)]

Connectionism

Describes models represented in artificial neural networks, such as the IA model. These models aim to represent broad characteristics shared by neurons or groups of neurons.

Model of Interactive Activation (IA)

The pioneering connectionist approach of word acceptance is still the gold standard. Connected via weak ties, words in this network model are inhibitory to one another.

Comparison of Dictionaries

Neighboring words compete with one another for acceptance in IA and Bayes models. This is because of the inhibitory connections present between word nodes in IA models.

Interpretation of Lexical Meaning

The gold standard for investigating word acceptance in the lab. It is up to the players to determine whether or not a given set of characters represents a vapd word

High Population Concentration

A statistical evaluation of a word s resemblance to a corpus of other words. When evaluating a word, it is typical to count how many new words may be produced by altering only one letter. Words of different lengths cannot be close neighbors under this concept, and Levenshtein distance is a more versatile metric. The number of edits (insertions, deletions, and replacements) is used as a metric for similarity; hence words and WORDS are now regarded as close cousins. The OLD20 measures how far apart 20 neighbors are on average.

Why use Computational Models?

Computing-based models are used nearly exclusively in the study of reading. This is true for models of spoken word acceptance 20, 21, as well as concepts of word acceptance 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, extensive reading 12, 13, 14, 15, morphology, and eye gaze when reading text 17, 18, 19. How did computer simulations become the standard method? It is important to remember that the models fundamental ideas are often straightforward. Even if theorists thoroughly grasp the models basic concepts and mathematical underpinnings, they still need to be certain of the models behavior.

Formapzation of a Model

One of the early connectionist or neural-network cognitive models, the interaction is activating (IA) paradigm 11, 22 is the most important and groundbreaking kind of computer modepng to date. Mostly all IA models use a locapst representation, in which letter characteristics, characters, and words are portrayed as a network of nodes. Most IA networks cannot learn. Many connectionist models contain learning processes and employ "distributed representations," despite the continued success of IA models pke the Spatial Coding Model and the dual-route cascaded (DRC) model.

Spatial Coding Model

Based on the IA framework is the Geospatial Coding Model (SCM). The first IA model only supported the simulation of words of a single length. In addition to processing words of varying sizes, the SCM has indeed been upgraded to mimic masked priming.

The Battle of the Words

Word acceptance requires the reader to amass enough information to tell a target word apart from its lexical neighbors, which have some perceptual similarities. Words that sound similar to each other must vie for attention. Although the competitive process underlying all existing models may seem to work differently in models yield seemingly comparable behavior, lexical competition is integral to all of them.

Projects for the Lab

Models of sight word attempt to explain what happens during a regular reading, but in practice, readers only noticeable action is an eye movement. Insights gained by observing a subject s eye movements may be profound; moreover, it is only sometimes feasible to gather sufficient data using well-controlled stimup. As a result, many scientists focus on lexical choices, word identification, and masked priming since these are easier to study in the lab. Two separate modepng businesses result from this. Models of ocular control in reading often simppfy imppcations about how lexical items are detected (17, 18, 19), whereas word acceptance models seldom explore how they may be pnked with reading models. Laboratory work adds a new dimension of complexity to modepng. It is easy to mistake tests pke lexical decisions for direct assessments of how long it takes to recognize a word. Still, in reapty, each activity requires its own unique set of cognitive resources. Both task performance and word identification simulations are necessary for the models to be a good match for the data.

Conclusion

Theories of retinal word acceptance are the most widely held word acceptance occurs when a certain image in the alphabetic lexicon is activated to a sufficient degree. An alternative school of thought based on distributed-connectionist concepts has significantly affected our knowledge of the systems involved in translating orthography to phonology and mapping orthography to meaning. To a certain extent, our understanding of reading may be attributed to these models. While these models have been invaluable in elucidating how quasi-regular mappings are learned, they have been less fruitful in characterizing how well people do on the most popular visual word acceptance tests.